**ABSTRACT NOT FOR CITATION WITHOUT AUTHOR PERMISSION. The title, authors, and abstract for this completion report are provided below. For a copy of the full completion report, please contact the author via e-mail at mwagner@msu.edu. Questions? Contact the GLFC via email at frp@glfc.org or via telephone at 734-662-3209.**
Altering
migratory routes of sea lamprey through push-pull application of semiochemicals
C. Michael Wagner1, Gregory J.
Byford1, James R. Miller2, Muraleedrahan
Nair3
Michigan State University
East Lansing, MI 48824
1Department
of Fisheries and Wildlife, 480 Wilson Rd., Rm. 13
2Department
of Entomology, 578 Wilson Rd., Rm. 203
3Department
of Horticulture, 1066 Bogue St., Rm. A420
December 2016
ABSTRACT:
The act of
migration often requires prey to move into regions of heightened predation
risk. During the return migration in the Great Lakes, the sea lamprey (Petromyzon
marinus) transitions from lakes to rivers,
choosing habitat in response to the presence of odor emitted by stream-resident
larval lampreys. This odor represents an ecological legacy, advertising the
location of high-quality spawning and rearing habitat to the non-homing migrants
seeking to reproduce. When attacked or killed during the migration, the sea
lamprey releases a powerful alarm cue; a chemical substance of unknown
structure that alerts conspecifics to the presence of predation risk. Previous
research has established that each odor, acting singly, is used to guide the
selection of movement paths in streams. The larval odor attracts migrants to
the side of the river associated with good habitat, the alarm cue repels them
from areas associated with risk. The goal of this project was to begin to map
how migrating sea lamprey respond to the odors in concert, both when
overlapping (competing information) and when spatially separated (contrasting
information). Two general research questions were addressed. First, will the
sea lamprey respond to the alarm cue in a threat-sensitive manner (increasing
avoidance with increasing concentration) such that a river containing larval
odor may be chemically ‘blocked’ by the application of a suitably-high
concentration of alarm cue? We tested this response in a natural stream
containing a population of larval sea lamprey. In the presence of larval odor
upstream, no application of the alarm cue was sufficient to halt upstream
migration. There was evidence that the animals slowed their upstream movement,
but the decision to continue migrating was unaffected (Manuscript #1). Second,
we examined the effects of five contrasting chemical information scenarios on
the decision to move upstream, side of the channel selected for movement, and
the timing of movement, as follows: (1) larval odor vs. river water
(opportunity only), (2) alarm cue vs. river water (avoidable risk, no opportunity),
(3) alarm cue vs. alarm cue (unavoidable risk, no opportunity), (4) alarm cue
vs. larval odor (avoidable risk coupled with opportunity), and (5) alarm cue +
larval odor vs. alarm cue (unavoidable risk coupled with opportunity). In this
experiment the migrating lampreys clearly modulated their upstream movement tendencies,
with the lowest movement during risk-only scenarios, and the greatest movement
with avoidable risk and opportunity, or opportunity alone (Manuscript #2).
Clearly, both larval odor and the alarm cue are used by the animal to select
risk-minimizing (or safe) routes towards habitat chemically labeled by living
larvae. While the alarm cue was a powerful motivator of movement paths, the
greatest effect was observed when the odors reinforce each other (e.g.,
attractant on one side, repellent on the other). Such an application (often
labeled ‘pushpull’) should prove highly effective in
guiding lampreys into target tributaries at confluences, or into areas targeted
for other means of control (e.g., traps). The project developed no evidence
that the alarm cue may be used to chemically block a stream in the presence of
larval odor. Finally, in a third experiment that arose opportunistically, we
discovered that the Pacific lamprey (Entosphenus
tridentatus) contains an alarm cue that is also
highly repellent to the sea lamprey, suggesting the species share components of
the cue. The sea lamprey may prove a useful surrogate species for studying
methods to conserve imperiled lampreys (Manuscript #3).